|
Rolf Reber (born 17 May 1959) is professor of psychology at the University of Oslo. Rolf Reber is known for his research on processing fluency, especially the processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure he developed together with Norbert Schwarz from the University of Michigan and Piotr Winkielman from the University of California at San Diego.〔Reber, R., Schwarz, N., Winkielman, P.: "Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience?", ''Personality and Social Psychology Review'', 8(4):364–382.〕 The core assumption of the theory is that an audience draws aesthetic pleasure from the fact that an object can be processed easily, especially if a viewer remains unaware of the source of this processing ease. This theory resolves an apparent contradiction between the uniformity of musical preferences in infants and the cultural differences of musical tastes in adults. Infants prefer consonant melodies because newborns share biological mechanisms that make them process consonance in music more easily than dissonance. When children grow up, they are exposed to the music of their culture, explaining why individuals from different cultures have different musical tastes. In addition, research found that processing fluency influences both affect〔Reber, R., Winkielman, P. & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. ''Psychological Science'', ''9'', 45–48.〕 and the judged truth of statements,〔Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. ''Consciousness and Cognition'', ''8'', 338–342.〕 suggesting that ease of processing is a common underlying experience in both perceived beauty and judged truth. This observation fits anecdotal observations that mathematicians and scientists sometimes use beauty of a theorem as an indication for its truth, an idea that has been explored in more recent work.〔Reber, R., Brun, M., & Mitterndorfer, K. (2008). The use of heuristics in intuitive mathematical judgment. ''Psychonomic Bulletin & Review'', ''15'', 1174–1178.〕 Processing fluency and its effects can help explain the "Aha"-experience. 〔Topolinski, S., & Reber, R. (2010). Gaining insight into the "Aha"-experience. ''Current Directions in Psychological Science'', 19, 402-405.〕〔Wray, H. (2011, January). Aha! The 23-Across Phenomenon. APS Observer, 24(1).〕The processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure has influenced work in psychology,〔Gazzaniga, M. S. (2008). ''Human: The Science Behind What Makes Us Unique''. New York: Ecco Books, Harper Collins.〕〔Rubin, M., Paolinia, S., & Crisp, R. J. (2010). A processing fluency explanation of bias against migrants. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 21–28.〕〔Topolinski , S. (2010). Moving the eye of the beholder: Motor components in vision determine aesthetic preference. ''Psychological Science'', ''21'', 1220–1224.〕 philosophy,〔Scharfstein, B.-A. (2009). ''Art without borders: A philosophical exploration of art and humanity''. Chicago: Chicago University Press.〕 marketing,〔Lee, A. Y., & Labroo, A. A. (2004). The Effect of Conceptual and Perceptual Fluency on Brand Evaluation. ''Journal of Marketing Research'', ''41'' (2004): 151–165.〕〔Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Of frogs, wines, and frowning watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. ''Journal of Consumer Research'', ''34'', 819–831.〕 and finance.〔Alter, A. L. & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Predicting short-term stock fluctuations by using processing fluency. ''Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences'', ''103'' (24), .〕 An extension of the processing fluency theory takes account of the fact that many artworks are difficult to process. Nevertheless, audiences interpret these artworks in a meaningful way and like them.〔Bullot, N. J., & Reber, R. (2013). The Artful Mind Meets Art History: Toward a Psycho-Historical Framework for the Science of Art Appreciation. ''Behavioral and Brain Sciences'', ''36'', 123-180.〕 More recently, Rolf Reber and his collaborators have developed and explored Example Choice, an instructional technique designed to increase relevance and student interest in the learning of abstract principles in mathematics and science. Students are given examples from different topics that all address the same underlying principle, and a student has to choose the example that interests him or her most. The chosen example is then used to explain the formal principle. This technique is supposed to connect the formal principle to students’ interest. Research has shown that students become more interested and spend more time learning the principle when they can choose an example than when they are given an example.〔Reber, R., Hetland, H., Chen, W., Norman, E., & Kobbeltvedt, T. (2009): ''Effects of example choice on interest, control, and learning.'' In: ''Journal of the Learning Sciences.'' 18 (4): 509–548.〕 Rolf Reber is author of two popular science books in German, among them “Kleine Psychologie des Alltäglichen” (A brief psychology of everyday life) which has been translated into Korean and Chinese.〔Reber, R. (2008). ''Kleine Psychologie des Alltäglichen'' (A brief psychology of everyday life), 2nd edition. München: C.H. Beck.〕 == References == 抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)』 ■ウィキペディアで「Rolf Reber」の詳細全文を読む スポンサード リンク
|